According to the developmental pathways model

Abstract

Developmental sequences in disruptive behavior from childhood to adolescence are traced retrospectively and prospectively in two community samples of boys. Three developmental pathways are distinguished: (a) an early authority conflict pathway, consisting in sequence of stubborn behavior, defiance, and authority avoidance; (b) a covert pathway, consisting of minor covert behaviors, property damage, and moderate to serious forms of delinquency; and (c) an overt pathway, consisting of aggression, fighting, and violence. The overlap among the three disruptive pathways is examined. Those boys who escalated in the overt pathway were more likely to escalate in the covert pathway than boys escalating in the covert pathway showing an escalation in the overt pathway. Escalation in the authority conflict pathway was not associated with escalation in either the overt or the covert pathways. Boys' rate of self-reported delinquency was highest for those in triple pathways (covert-overt-authority conflict) or in certain dual pathways (covert-overt, covert-authority conflict). However, by age 16 the highest rate of offending was displayed by those in the triple pathways. The rate of violent offenses was also highest for those in the triple pathways and for those in the overt and covert pathways. Results from the rate for court petitions largely supported these findings. Lowest rates of offending were observed for boys in the overt and authority conflict pathways. Implications are discussed for clinical practice and future research.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1993). Taxonomy and comorbidity of conduct problems: Evidence of empirically based approaches. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 5164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Ver-mont.Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1989). The permanence of change and the impermanence of stability. Human Development, 32, 187192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Belson, W. A. (Ed.). (1975). Juvenile theft: The causal factors. London: Harper & Row.Google Scholar

Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., Roth, J. A., & Visher, C. A. (Eds.). (1986). Criminal careers and ‘career criminals’. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D. (1990). An historical perspective on the discipline of developmental psychopathology. In Rolf, J., Masten, A., Cicchetti, D., Neuchterlein, K., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 228). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Costello, A. J., Edelbrock, C. S., Dulcan, M. K., Kalas, R., & Klaric, S. H. (1984). Report on the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC). Unpublished manuscript, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinical, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar

Donovan, J. E., Jessor, R., & Costa, F. M. (1988). Syndrome of problem behavior in adolescence: A replication. Journal of Consulting and Clinic Psychology, 56, 762765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. S. (1985). Explaining delinquency and drug use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar

Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., Elliott, D. S., Hawkins, J. D., Kandel, D. B., Klein, M. W., McCord, J., Rowe, D. C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1990). Advancing knowledge about the onset of delinquency and crime. In Lahey, B. B. & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 283342). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Freud, A. (1965). The concept of developmental lines. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 18, 245265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Green, S. M., Loeber, R., & Lahey, B. B. (1991) Stability of mother's recall of the age at onset of their child's attention and hyperactivity problems. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 135137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hamburg, B. A., Kraemer, H. C., & Jahnke, W. (1975). Behavioral and attitudinal correlates of substantial drug use. American Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 627647.Google ScholarPubMed

Huizinga, D., Esbensen, F. A., & Weiher, A. W. (1991). Are there multiple paths to delinquency? The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 82, 83118.Google Scholar

Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Kandel, D. B. (1978). Convergence in prospective longitudinal surveys of drug use in normal populations. In Kandel, D. B. (Ed.), Longitudinal research on drug use (pp. 338). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Kandel, D. B. (1980). Developmental stages in adoles-cent drug involvement. In Lettieri, D. J., Sayers, M., & Wallenstein Pearson, H. (Eds.), Theories on drug abuse. Selected contemporary perspectives. NIDA Research Monograph 30.Google Scholar

Kandel, D. B. & Faust, R. (1975). Sequence and stages in patterns of adolescent drug use. Archives in General Psychiatry, 32, 923932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kazdin, A. E. (1992). Overt and covert antisocial behavior: Child and family charateristics among psychiatric inpatient children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1, 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lahey, B. B., Frick, P. J., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L., Van Horn, Y., & Christ, M. A. G. (1992). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: A meta-analytic review of factor analyses and cross-validation in a clinic sample. Unpublished manu-script, University of Miami, Miami, FL.Google Scholar

Le Blanc, M., Cote, G., & Loeber, R. (1991). Temporal paths in delinquency: Stability, regression and progression analyzed with panel data from an adolescent and a delinquent male sample. Canadian Journal of Criminology, Jan., 2344.Google Scholar

Le Blanc, M., & Frechette, M. (1989). Male criminal activity from childhood to adulthood: Multilevel and developmental perspectives. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R. (1988). The natural histories of juvenile conduct problems, substance use and delinquency: Evidence for developmental progressions. In Lahey, B. B. & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 73124). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R. (1991) Questions and advances in the study of developmental pathways. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Develop-mental Psychopathology: III (pp. 97115). Rochester, NY: Rochester University Press.Google Scholar

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., & Lahey, B. B. (1990). Mental health professionals' perception of the utility of children, mothers, and teachers as informants on childhood psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 136143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. A. G., & Frick, P. J. (1992). Developmental sequences in the age of onset of disruptive child behaviors. Journal of Child and Family Studies 7, 2141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1989). Optimal informants on childhood disruptive behaviors. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 317337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R., Keenan, K., Giroux, B., Sieck, W., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Van Kammen, W. (1992). The differential role of early onset, hyperactivity, and substance use in boys'pathways to serious disruptive behavior. Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar

Loeber, R., & Le Blanc, M. (1990). Toward a develop-mental criminology. In & (Eds.), Crime and justice, an annual review (Vol. 12, pp. 375473). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Loeber, R., Russo, M. F., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Lahey, B. B. (1992, 10 19–20). Relationship between internalizing symptoms and pathways in disruptive child behavior. Paper presented at the N.I.M.H. Workshop on Affective Processes in Adolescence,Bethesda, MD.Google Scholar

Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. B. (1985a). Empirical evidence for overt and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 337352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. B. (1985b). The utility of differentiating between mixed and pure forms of antisocial child behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 315336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, R., Van Kammen, W. B., & Farrington, D. P. (1989). Development of a new measure of self-reported antisocial behavior in young children: Prevalence and reliability. In Klein, M. W. (Ed.), Self-report methodology in criminological research (pp. 203225). Boston: Kluwer-Nijhoff.Google Scholar

Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Van Kammen, W. B., & Farrington, D. P. (1991). Initiation, escalation and desistance in juvenile offending and their correlates. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 82, 3682.Google Scholar

Maguin, E. (1992). Manual for retrieving juvenile court data files from the Allegheny County Juve-nile Court Files. Unpublished manuscript, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E. (1992). Adolescent-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison.Google Scholar

Patterson, G. R. (1980). Children who steal. In Hirschi, T. & Gottfredson, M. (Eds.), Understanding crime: Current theory & research (pp. 7390). London: Sage.Google Scholar

Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: Vol. 3. Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia Publishing.Google Scholar

Patterson, G. R. (1986). Performance models for anti-social boys. American Psychologist, 41, 432444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Pulkkinen, L., & Tremblay, R. E. (in press) Patterns of boys' social adjustment in two cultures and at different ages: A longitudinal perspective. International Journal of Behavioural Development.Google Scholar

Radke-Yarrow, M., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1970). Reliability of maternal retrospection: A preliminary report. In Danziger, K. (Ed.), Readings in child socialization. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar

Russo, M., Loeber, R., & Keenan, K. (1992). Oppositional defiant and conduct disorders: Validation of the proposed DSM-IV alternative diagnostiction. Unpublished manuscript, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar

Tolan, P. H., & Guerra, N. G. (1992). A developmental perspective on adolescent antisocial behavior. Unpublished paper, University of Illinois at Chicago.Google Scholar

Tremblay, R. E. (1992). The prediction of delinquent behavior from childhood behavior: Personality theory revisited. In McCord, J. (Ed.), Facts, frame-works, and forecasts (Vol. 3, pp. 193230). New Brunswick, NJ: Transactions.Google Scholar

Van Kammen, W. B., Loeber, R., & StoutharnerLoeber, M. (1991) Substance use, antisocial, and delinquent behavior in young boys. Youth and Adolescence, 20, 445451.Google Scholar

What are the developmental pathways?

A developmental pathway is defined as the orderly behavioral development between more than two problem behaviors with individuals differing in their propensity to progress along the successive problem behavior represented by the pathway during development.

What are the 3 pathways to delinquency?

Three developmental pathways are distinguished: (a) an early authority conflict pathway, consisting in sequence of stubborn behavior, defiance, and authority avoidance; (b) a covert pathway, consisting of minor covert behaviors, property damage, and moderate to serious forms of delinquency; and (c) an overt pathway, ...

What is multiple pathway model?

Multiple pathways are defined as a variety of structured academic opportunities for students to achieve their goal for high school graduation and postsecondary success. Each pathway is defined by its programming and is accessed by each student based on the individual student's academic interest and unique needs.

What specific age exists the authority of conflict pathways to delinquency or having stubborn behavior?

The Authority Conflict Pathway, which starts with stubborn behavior before age 12 and progresses to defiance and then to authority avoidance (e.g., truancy). The Covert Pathway, which starts with minor covert acts before age 15 and progresses to property damage and then to moderate and then to serious delinquency.